SimplyBrainy » Perceptual Skills Curriculum Research

Perceptual Skills Curriculum Research

The Development and Validation

of an Individualized

Perceptual Skills Curriculum

Jerome Rosner

Learning Research and Development Center

University of Pittsburgh

1972

Download the PDF ebook

The research reported herein was supported by the Ford Foundation and the Learning Research and Development Center supported in part as a research and development center by funds from the United States Office of Education, Department of Health, Education, and Welfare. The opinions expressed in this publication do not necessarily reflect the position or policy of the Ford Foundation or of the Office of Education and no official endorsement should be inferred.

Acknowledgement

Although this paper shows only one author, it represents the efforts of the total staff of the Perceptual Skills Curriculum Project as well as assistance from teachers and LRDC colleagues, too numerous to list.

The following have worked on this Project, at one time or another, during the past three years:

Joanne Gass; Dorothy Hurst; Joan Goodnight; Sheila Levine; Sharyn Nassau; Dorothea Simon; Susan Tutko; Rebecca Wiegers

INTRODUCTION

Developers of instructional programs must make certain assump­tions about the entering abilities of the student populations for whom their programs are intended. A significant proportion of such assumptions are related to the students’ basic information processing skills. For example, every first-grade instructional program for unimpaired, or so-called “normal, children- -children whose chronological and mental ages approxi­mate 6 years; whose visual and hearing acuities are within normal ranges; who can speak intelligibly and manipulate a pencil with some control- -assumes that the students already know, or will readily learn, the basic visu~ and acoustical communication codes used in that program. Indi­vidual differences in students’ cognitive abilities are anticipated; indeed, provisions may be built into the instructional program to accommodate these differences. On the other hand, individual differences in the basic processes of organizing the sensory data–the media from which meaning­ful visual and acoustical information is constructed–are usually of less concern. The inference is that, if the children meet the criteria of ‘un­impaired,” they will possess these basic aptitudes.

Unfortunately, such assumptions are false. During the past decade, for example, much attention has been given to two separate groups of chil­dren who though ‘unimpaired,” enter first grade unprepared to deal with symbols in a reliable and efficient manner; one group has been categorized as “learning disabled”; the other as “culturally disadvantaged.

Children with learning disabilities have been the concern of many investigators (see, for example, Kaluger & Kolson, 1969; Frierson & Barbe, 1967; McCarthy & McCarthy, 1969), including a special Task Force sponsored by the National Institutes of Health (Clements, 1966; Clements, 1969; Chalfont & Scheffelin, 1969). So far, however, there is little agreement as to cause, treatment, or even an appropriate descrip­tive name for this group. In addition to “learning disabled,” they have been called perceptually handicapped, minimally brain damaged, children with cerebral dysfunction, neurologically impaired, and dyslexic. (For an extended list, see Clements, 1966.) What agreement there is centers on the identification of certain behavioral characteristics that tend to be common to the group, including indications of perceptual dysfunction. These children usually display all of the attributes of the “unimpaired, yet they do not profit from instruction in a normal fashion.

The second group, the “culturally disadvantaged,” has also been the concern of a number of scientists (see, for example, Bereiter & Engelmann, 1966; Deutsch, 1965). These children, too, often show ~gns of perceptual dysfunction. The symbols of the classroom tend to cause them confusion. They, also, fail to profit from academic instruction in an expected manner.

There are, of course, basic differences between the two groups:  socioeconomic status, for one; and the causal factors that are usually related to their learning problem, for another. Middle-class children who show unpredicted achievement problems are labeled as learning disabled, and the speculation about cause centers around central nervous system dysfunction. On the other hand, children from families of lower socioeconomic status who perform poorly in the classroom are generally characterized as culturally disadvantaged; the inferred cause here is re­lated primarily to experiential factors. Either group, alone, is of suf­ficient size to cause concern; combined, their numbers are significant indeed.

Acknowledging this situation, and being committed to a concept of “adaptive education” (Glaser, 1971), the Learning Research and Develop­ment Center at the University of Pittsburgh initiated an effort in 1969–the Perceptual Skills Curriculum Project–to study the problem. Adaptive education, in this context, indicates recognition of the educator’s respon­sibility to teach each child, and that academic failure, if it does occur, is to be attributed to the educator–not to the child. Therefore, such categorizations as “learning disabled,” “culturally disadvantaged,” or what have you, are really irrelevant, since they do not provide useful information about the individual child’s educational needs; indeed, they tend to be useful only in that they can serve to absolve mainstream education from the responsibility of teaching such children.

The adaptive model provides the educator with two options, both of which may be applied: (1) attempt to define the optimum instructional program for each child; (2) provide the child with the training needed to acquire those skills that are prerequisite to successful performance in a specific instructional program.

Insofar as the first alternative is concerned, Glaser (1971) cor­rectly points out that “there are few alternative paths through elementary school.” The first alternative, therefore, appears to be ruled out for that age group. Perhaps, then, yet a third option should be proposed. That is, if a child does not manifest the abilities prerequisite to achiev­ing the academic objectives of an elementary school curriculum, he should be placed in other, non-academic programs. This approach is commonly used with the mentally retarded. Maybe such an approach is justifiable for mentally retarded children; maybe it is not. One fact is clear, however. With children of normal intelligence, such an approach is utterly inappropriate. As Resnick (1972) points out:

…”in urbanized and industrialized societies, there is probably no more important skill in gaining control over one’s own life than reading and the associated skills of literacy. If this is so, then ‘respecting’ an individual’s freedom to not learn to read actually means condemning him to a life of bondage.”

If we accept Resnick’s statement–and it is very difficult to reject–we must rule Out, a priori, the third alternative. The Perceptual Skills Curriculum Project directed its attention to the second option: the modi­fication of prerequisite abilities through training.

PROJECT GOALS

The Project organized its task into four major goals:

  1. Identify those perceptual skills that appear to be directly re­lated to the basic classroom tasks of reading and arithmetic at the primary level.
  2. Given identified perceptual skills that do relate directly to classroom achievement, determine whether such skills can be trained effectively.
  3. Given trainable skills that are relevant to classroom achieve­ment, determine whether the effect of that training can be measured in the classroom behaviors. In other words, can transfer be effected?
  4. Given affirmative responses to all of the above, describe the training in a way that will allow it to be implemented and man­aged in the classroom of a public school, as a Perceptual Skills Curriculum,

During the past three years, a series of research and development studies has been conducted in an attempt to achieve these goals.  Basic mechanisms have not been explored; rather, a theme of empirical valida­tion of clinical procedures has been followed. To a large degree, the goals have been attained; it is the purpose of this paper to describe the methods employed, the information gathered, and the current status of the Project. Before doing so, however, it will be helpful to state the rationale on which this Project was based.

The rationale was originally stated in a preliminary document: a working paper description of the projected design of an individualized Perceptual Skills Curriculum (Rosner, 1969). The following statement is not identical to that original statement. Three years of research have effected certain changes in concepts. The basic premises, however, appear to have been supported and are retained.

RATIONALE

An Operational Definition of Perceptual Skills

Perceptual skills, in the context of this paper, refers to the be­havioral processes of analyzing and organizing raw sensory data–the “stuff” of communication–into meaningful symbolic units. Perceptual information, then, refers to the raw sensory data; symbolic information refers to specific units of perceptual data to which society has given a construct value. To make clear the distinction between perceptual and symbolic information, look at the two “messages” shown in Figure 1.

Figure 1

Figure1

Both are constructed of “stuff”–visual sensations–in the form of ink traces on paper. Hence, both contain perceptual information.  Both also contain symbolic information, but our ability to gain access to that information is directly dependent upon our familiarity with the symbols and the lan­guages to which they belong, Both are, in fact, conveying the same in­formation; both refer to the same animal. One, of course, employs the symbols of the English language; the other uses Chinese symbols.  To the viewer who is familiar with the printed English language, the word “cat” is symbolic data, constructed from perceptual information. He will perceive it either as a word or as a series of letters, dependent upon his reading skills; once he has learned to discriminate the letters, he is not likely to perceive it simply as a pattern of interrelated lines.

On the other hand, if he is not also familiar with printed Chinese language symbols, he will treat those as perceptual data; an assortment of lines that create a graphic pattern. His unit of analysis will be the line rather than the symbol. The reader of English, when asked to copy the word “cat,” will do just that–copy the series of letters that comprise the word “cat.” When asked to copy the Chinese written representation of the word “cat,” however, he will attempt to replicate a graphic pat­tern–he will draw interrelated lines rather than copy a visual represen­tation of a verbal construct.

The opposite situation will exist for the individual who is familiar with the written Chinese language but not the English. He will view the Chinese symbols for what they are–organized visual representations of spoken language. To him it is the English symbols that will be viewed as a graphic pattern–an organization of visual sensations that lack sym­bolic content.

A similar contrast can be made between perceptual and symbolic acoustical information. Consider the same two messages in spoken form: /cat/ and /mou/. When spoken to a user of the English language, the former is readily perceived as a meaningful single unit–a construc­tion of sequenced phonic events that conveys meaning. In contrast, the same sounds–”cat”–spoken to a user of the Chinese language who is not also familiar with spoken English, will be processed as perceptual information–as a pattern of sounds that lacks meaning and perhaps even precise organization.

How does this pertain to children’s school performance, when only one symbol system is involved? The classroom typically provides an environment where meaningful information is generated, transmitted, received, and hopefully assimilated by the students and their teacher Figure 2 illustrates, in general, the basic behaviors assumed from “unimpaired” children who have been placed in such an environment. Information, in the form of visual and acoustical sensations, is generated and/or presented by the teacher. The child is expected to organize these into matching, meaningful constructs: visually, as pictures or alphanu­meric symbols; acoustically, as phonological constructs–that is, spoken words. Simply stated, even though he may not interpret the information correctly, the child is expected to “see”–perceive–what the teacher is showing him and “hear”–perceive–what the teacher is saying to him. In addition, if he is to meet the demands of a normal classroom, he is expected to display little difficulty in learning to produce visual and acoustical symbols that match those produced by his teacher and class­mates. That is, he is expected to be able to “write” that which he is shown, and “say” that which he is told.  Further, he must learn to repre­sent sounds graphically (hear and write), and to represent visual symbols acoustically (see and say). To do all this, the child must understand the construction of the visual and acoustical symbols. To understand their construction, he must be able to sort out and order the sensory elements–the “stuff”–from which they were formed. He must “perceive” first, if he is to “understand.”

Figure 2

Figure2

Not only must the student be able to perform all of these acts; he must be able to perform them easily, quickly, almost automatically. If the perceptual acts require great conscious effort, it follows that there will be less time and energy available for the interpretive aspects of the task. It is analogous to the conditions facing the student driver during his first driving lesson. He can see and hear well; his “equipment” is intact–it has been checked. He is sufficiently intelligent and adequately strong. Yet, during this first driving lesson, such an excessive amount of energy is devoted to survival–to controlling the automobile–that it is likely that his peripheral visual fields are effectively constricted; that he hears less keenly; that indeed he is operationally less “intelligent.” He survives; he learns to drive.  The behaviors that demanded so much conscious effort at first now become almost automatic. He can see, hear, and think more effectively while driving. He has learned to use his equip­ment efficiently. He has linked together a series of relatively small, segmented behaviors into larger and fewer units. He need not devote attention to a myriad of separate independent actions. He can now function on a higher level of organization.

A child entering the first grade with inefficient perceptual skills is in much the same situation as the novice driver. He is expected to receive and produce the sensory codes of the classroom reliably and efficiently. To be able to perform the behaviors reliably is not enough; unless they are also performed efficiently–almost automatically–there will be little time or energy available to sustain at and master the inter­pretive aspects of the task; and yet it is to these interpretive acts that his teacher will look when assessing the child’s achievement. It is not enough to be able to “do it if he tries.” One doesn’t have limitless re­sources–the child cannot always “try hard. ” The result–probable failure, and worse yet, accusations that he was capable but just didn’t try! This is particularly important in that the nature of the perceptual tasks presented to the first-grade child becomes more and more complex almost on a day-to-day basis. Hence, the child who “could do it if he tries” and did try to a sufficient degree during his first month in school, may very well find himself in an impossible situation in subsequent months, as the perceptual demands increase faster than does his ability to meet them.

For example, the child’s first pre-primer and workbook pages present very simple formats; there are just a few large letters on a page. Why? Not because six-year-old children cannot see smaller print but, rather, because the publishers are acknowledging that the child is young and, hence, inexperienced in organizing complex visual presenta­tions. Very quickly, though, the situation changes. Presentations do become more complex; letter size shrinks, and more print appears on a page.  The perceptual tasks steadily become more demanding. It is assumed that the child’s perceptual skills will be appropriate to this task as well as to the many other tasks that are presented daily in the class­room.

The assumption is correct for the majority of “unimpaired” chil­dren in the first grade; it is false, however, for a very significant num­ber of their classmates (Rosner, Richman, & Scott, 1969).  These are the ones who, in varying degrees, are less capable than expected in one or more of the performance skills described above: that is, in receiving and/or producing visual and/or acoustical sensations in a reliable and efficient manner. They can hear, see, speak, and manipulate a pencil, but their inefficiency in analyzing and organizing the sensations of the classroom into symbolic constructs prevents them from performing accurately, at the expected rate. They learn less of that which the teach­er is teaching — they are underachievers. In one socioeconomic setting, they may be known as “learning disabilities”; in another, they are “cul­turally disadvantaged”; in either, they may be termed “lazy.”

It is reasonable to assume that perceptual skills are developed, determined by both the child’s biological integrity and his post-natal ex­periences. How do they develop? What experiences affect them? Two general principles of development apply. One, that all acquired motor functions proceed along a continuum from global to differentiated (Espen­schade & Eckert, 1967). Secondly, that over time, the child’s ability to analyze and order visual and acoustical data is less dependent upon the tactile-kinesthetic supporting cues derived from these motor functions.

In regard to the first principle, consider the motor skills of the neonate.  In his first months of life he cannot voluntarily move one of his legs or arms without making compensatory, reflexive movements with the other parts of the body (Gesell, 1952).  By the time he enters first grade, if development has proceeded normally, he will be able to perform such discrete acts as balancing, hopping on one foot, and skip­ping, all of which indicate that he has reached a level on the Global Differentiated scale where he has sorted out many of his body parts, and his motor system is capable of reorganizing them in a variety of dynamic patterns.

Similar changes can be observed in prehension and vocalization skills. The global, reflexive grasp of the neonate develops into the palmar grasp of the one-year-old. Upon entry into first grade, the child will probably have sorted out the basic components of his hands–his fingers–to the point where he now has available a splendidly designed tool for ex­ploring and manipulating his world in a very refined manner, as well as for controlling the movements of a writing tool efficiently–with two or three fingers of one hand (Gesell et al., 1940). In comparable fashion, the infant’s undifferentiated cry is replaced by the toddler’s initial vocabulary of a few words which, in turn, is expanded and refined into the appro­priately articulated speech patterns of the first-grader (Lenneberg, 1967).

All of these changes appear to be the outcomes of normal growth and development. The extent to which they have occurred is, in fact, part of that which is probed when one attempts to assess maturation. If a six-year-old is significantly awkward, if he grips a pencil with four or five fingers instead of two, if his speech patterns reveal traits more typical of the four-year-old, he is described as “immature”–at least in respect to these specific behaviors; behaviors that are critically related to the classroom tasks he will be expected to accomplish.

Insofar as the second developmental principle is concerned–that is, the child’s relative dependency on related tactile-kinesthetic cues in analyzing visual and acoustical data, it has been noted that “Perception is influenced by motor activity from the outset, just as the latter is by the former (Piaget, 1960, p. 87),” and that conceiving of sensory processes as independent of motor processes is “fallacious.” The one-year-old appears to analyze his visual world more reliably if he is able to con­firm that which he sees with some physical contact (White, 1970). As the child develops, he depends less upon overt tactile-kinesthetic infor­mation to confirm visual experiences. He acquires the ability to explore and organize with his eyes; the tactile-kinesthetic involvement becomes covert–it is internalized.  In a sense, his hands have “taught” his eyes; he now can function as though imaginary hands extended from his eyes. He perceives visual data as though he were overtly manipulating it. He can explore vaster expanses of space with his eyes than with his hands–and do it much more rapidly, efficiently–clearly an advantage insofar as classroom performance is concerned. In effect, sensory-MOTOR functions have become SENSORY-motor, as an outcome of normal devel­opment.  There is still a motor component in the behavior; it merely is less obvious (Zinchenko, 1970).

The motor component can and will become overt if the child-­or, indeed, the adult–encounters sufficient stress to necessitate analysis of the sensations at a lower level of organization. One need only watch the five-year-old learn to count objects. Initially, he learns to touch the objects with his fingers as he simultaneously states the numerals. In time, he “touches” with his eyes and commences to perceive groupings of objects–subsets within the whole. Overt hand involvement is not needed unless he is presented with an array of objects that is not ordered; where they are scattered randomly within a field. In such situations, overt hand use–self-produced cues such as pointing, and perhaps even touching–will probably be evidenced.

A similar phenomenon may be noted in the sensory-motor behaviors related to speech and audition. The vocal mechanism, as it relates to audition, is in this sense the counterpart to the hand as it relates to vision. Until the child learns to mediate sounds visually, vocalization is the only reliable method he has for exploring what he has heard. As he matures, the child depends less upon explicit vocalization for analyzing spoken sounds he develops the ability to analyze spoken sounds covertly–as though he had stated them aloud. Hence, again we observe a shifting from sensory-MOTOR to SENSORY-motor; and again, given a series of spoken sounds that are not familiar to the listener–be he adult or child–­one will note manifestation of overt or sub-vocalization as a tangible means for analyzing that which has been heard.

Developmental studies (Zinchenko, 1970) have shown that these two basic transformations: “Global –> Differentiated” and “sensory­-MOTOR –> SENSORY-motor,” occur simultaneously. Indeed, they tend to be mutually reinforcing. The acquisition of more articulate man­ual and vocal motor skills provides for more precise exploration of visual and acoustical sensory data which, in turn, allows for more differentiated visual and auditory analysis behaviors that depend less and less upon tangible, tactile-kinesthetic confirmation as higher levels of organization are achieved. These more elaborate receptor skills then serve to monitor the vocal and digital manipulative skills more precisely, and so on.

Hand and eye, speech mechanism and ear, function as though linked in a continuous loop, each component serving to refine the process of the other through feedback. Ultimately, as proposed above, overt ex­plorations of visual and acoustical data become redundant and inefficient. Overt behaviors then become covert; explicit tactile-kinesthetic involve­ment becomes implicit. Thus the child acquires the basic information processing competencies needed to analyze and synthesize visual and auditory communications–the aptitudes assumed by the instructional programs of his classroom.

Visual-Motor Skills Development

What are these skills? It is easier to describe, first, how they are tested. Many visual-motor tests are limited to discrimination re­sponses in which the child is asked to indicate whether two graphic patterns are, or are not, identical. Since appropriate classroom perfor­mance requires the child to produce perceptual constructs as well as receive them, discrimination tasks, as customarily designed, provide in­sufficient information. The ability to recognize similarities and differences in visual patterns is not equivalent to knowing the construction of patterns well enough to produce them accurately. Copying tests, therefore, serve our needs better.

Gesell (1940), Starr (1961), Bender (1938), and others have shown that most children, as they mature, are able to copy more complex geo­metric designs. As such, designers of primary-grade instructional pro­grams logically assume that it is feasible to employ visual stimuli of comparable complexity.

What is involved in a copying task? What does the “good” copier know that distinguishes him from the “poor ” copier? For example, why does one six-year-old copy the asterisk, an item in the Rutgers Drawing Test (Starr, 1961), relatively accurately, while his classmate, also six years old, reveals gross inadequacy in the same task? Figure 3 portrays two contrasting responses.

Figure 3

Figure3

Does child A literally see more clearly than child B–is that the cause of B’s inadequate response? That is not very likely. The rela­tionship between visual acuity and copying skills is quite low (Rosner et al. , 1969). What else then? Distorted vision? — does child B literally see the stimulus to be the way he has drawn it? Again, one cannot find support for such a notion in the currently available data. Poor drawing skills? –does child B draw what he does because he cannot control the pencil? This is indeed quite possible. Motor skills, however, can be tested with simpler forms and, in most instances, do not appear to be the cause of poor copying skills.

Why, then? The reasonable proposition is that child A perceives the stimulus to be constructed of a finite number of elements (lines) that interrelate in a specific way. Child B, on the other hand, perceives the stimulus to be constructed of a non-specific number of elements (lines) that interrelate in an imprecise manner. Child B views the asterisk much like an adult views a tree in full leaf, or any other complex visual construction. Given the task of “copying” a tree in full leaf, the adult will sketch, or represent, the trunk, branches, and leaves; under normal cir­cumstances he cannot possible replicate the details–there simply are too many and their interrelationships are too intricate.

Child B’s response indicates a representation rather than a repli­cation. As he acquires the capacity to “see” a finite number of lines and understand their interrelationships, he will more closely approximate a replication of the stimulus design. Such a capacity seems to depend upon certain basic skills. To “see” a finite number of lines implies a prior ability to sort out the elements–the salient attributes–of the design, either by discriminating them individually or by recognizing sub-assemblies of elements within the total pattern. At whatever level, the “sorting out” process can be enhanced by overt tactile-kinesthetic involvement (e.g., by tracing over the lines), and by verbal mediation (e.g., by counting the lines, by identifying them according to their spatial orientations, or by naming sub-assemblies–such as the + that is embedded in the asterisk–that have been recognized within the total configuration).

To order those elements requires an additional skill–the child must be able to view the lines as though they were plotted on a map of spatial coordinates. For example, consider the effect of presenting the same task–copying the asterisk–with one exception. The addition of two spatial coordinates–one vertical, one horizontal–superimposed on the stimulus, and a matching set of coordinates on the response space. (See Figure 4.)

Figure 4

Figure4

Is it likely that the spatial interrelationships will be represented more accurately? Undoubtedly, given that the individual elements of the design had been recognized.

Let’s go one step further. Again the asterisk is presented for copying, but this time it is superimposed by three vertical and three horizontal coordinates; matching coordinates are provided in the response space. (See Figure 5.)

Figure 5

Figure5

Does the task of replication become less difficult? Certainly–much additional organizational support has been provided.

Yet one more step. Again the asterisk–this time it is superim­posed by five vertical and five horizontal coordinates, with matching coordinates provided in the response space. (See Figure 6.)

Figure 6

Figure6

In this last format, the spatial interrelationships are fully dis­played. All cues are overt–nothing need be inferred.

Now, look at the original asterisk and describe it verbally. You will use such spatial terms as “middle,” “above, ” “half-way, ” “to the right,” and so forth; terms that have no pertinence unless the space to which they relate has been defined by an implicit spatial coordinate map. It is reasonable, therefore, to assume that the copier who represents spatial inter relationships accurately, without additional organizational cues, is inferring such a map, using whatever other salient cues are available–such as the topological cues provided by the edges of the paper, and so forth.

In brief review, then, it has been proposed that growth and develop­ment normally provides the child with the capacity to analyze and repro­duce increasingly complex visual constructs, wherein the unit of analysis–the salient attributes of the construct–also becomes increasingly com­plex and the organizers increasingly effective. Thus, there is less de­pendency on overt tactile-kinesthetic involvement with the visual data as well as less dependency on overt organizational cues within the stimulus field itself. To test a child’s visual-motor skills means scaling his per­formance along three dimensions: If his sensory-motor skills are appro­priately differentiated for his age, and if he displays no more need for overt tactile-kinesthetic cues and overt spatial cues than do his age peers, he will be judged as having demonstrated “normal” visual-motor skills. If his performance is less than satisfactory, the problem becomes one of determining where he was lacking–along which dimension(s) and to what degree.

Auditory-Motor Skills Development

Again, it is easier first to describe how auditory-motor skills are tested. Consider what is asked by most currently popular auditory perception tests. These, almost invariably, are discrimination tests that either ask for “same-different” responses to pairs of words, or ask the child to choose, from an array of three or four words, the two words that begin or end with the same consonant sound. None of these ask the testee to produce an acoustical construct; thus, none of these satisfy our criteria. Slingerland (1962) comes closer to our needs in the Echolalia subtest of her Test for Specific Language Disabilities. In this subtest, the child is asked to repeat certain words–words that are more likely to reveal auditory reception or production sequencing confusions; for example, such words as: hospital (e.g., hopsital), spaghetti (e.g., piz­ghetti), animal (e.g., aminal), and philosophy (e.g., phisolody). Unfor­tunately, the Echolalia subtest does not seem to discriminate well among primary-grade children unless their problem is severe. In addition, the subtest contains a distinct bias in favor of the child whose background has exposed him to the test item words.

There will be extensive discussion regarding a more useful instru­ment later in this paper. In the meantime, what does inadequate perfor­mance on an auditory perceptual task indicate? Poor hearing acuity?–only rarely. Does the child literally not hear the discrete differences between two very similar words?– such a statement would be very diffi­cult to support with available clinical audiological data. What, then? Re­call the illustration offered in the discussion of visual-motor skills. An inadequate visual perceiver was described as one who did not recognize that a visual construction was made up of a finite number of elements that interrelated in a specific manner. The description also fits the in­adequate auditory perceiver. To illustrate the situation, state aloud:

“Please sit down.”

Although speakers’ styles vary, it is probable that the words were generated as “pleasesitdown”–a continuous stream of sounds. Spoken phrases, though composed of separate words, are rarely uttered in a way that acknowledges their separateness. They usually are blended together into a series of connected sounds. The responsibility for analyzing and organizing that stream of sounds into a series of words almost invariably falls to the listener. If that task cannot be perceived appropriately, the listener may be in much the same position as the in­dividual who hears, for the first time, the once popular song:

“mareseat­oatsanddoeseatoatsandlittlelambseativy.”

As a stream of unorganized, meaningless sounds, recalling them is exceedingly difficult, if in fact even possible. If they cannot be recalled, then surely they cannot be organized according to semantic attributes; hence, meaningful informa­tion cannot be readily extracted from the sensations.

Most first-grade teachers have observed this phenomenon. Many a child has entered the first-grade classroom, having learned the “Alpha­bet Song, ” perceiving “lmnop” as a single letter. The anecdotes about similar confusions with some of the phrases of the Lord’s Prayer, the Pledge of Allegiance, and other verbal presentations that were learned by rote, are ubiquitous. In most instances, the situation is benign; the re­sult of a simple confusion that will not have a detrimental effect on learning. In some situations, however, the problem may be critical, and difficult to deal with because of the way in which acoustical information is typically presented, in contrast to visual.

Visual representations of speech, such as this printed sentence, follow certain rules. Individual manuscript letters are separated by small spaces; larger spaces separate the words. Capital letters begin sentences; periods end sentences. A second line of print parallels the first line; the third follows the pattern, and so on. Order is maintained; organization is provided. The visual sensations are treated in an organ­ized way that enhances the viewer’s chances for extracting information from the symbols. The burden for organizing a visual presentation does not rest solely on the perceiver; the presenter always shares in the task.

The rules are less precise and rigid in presenting acoustical messages. Certainly, the sounds must follow in some specific sequence, but they need not be organized to the same level as visual material. Much more variance is tolerated. Reflect upon the variety of speaking styles and speech patterns to which a child may be exposed in a school building!

It seems reasonable to suggest that the assumptions made regard­ing visual perception are applicable to auditory processing as well. To satisfy the auditory perceptual demands of a primary-grade instructional program, the child must recognize that spoken phrases consist of a finite series of phonic elements (salient attributes) that interrelate or occur in a specific way. Indeed, to master the basic decoding skills of reading, he must discriminate these phonic elements at the phoneme, rather than the word, level and be aware of the specific sequential relationships of those phonemes in a spoken word.

He must not only appreciate that the word “cat” is constructed with three visual symbols, but that its acousti­cal construction is the product of three blended phonemes. It is true, of course, that children can and do memorize a limited number of words; sight reading vocabularies are important and within the repertory of all readers. However, no one can memorize all of the printed words of the language. At some point in the primary grades, regardless of the nature of the reading program used, sight-sound relationships must be developed at the phoneme level.

Acoustical events, such as occur in speech, do not have spatial attributes. As such, the salient cues that, in a visual presentation, facilitate the inference of a spatial map of coordinates, are not pertinent to auditory processes. Phonic events occur along the dimension of time, not space. It is only when we represent speech with visual symbols that a spatial dimension is called for–one that varies according to the con­ventions of the specific culture (e.g., English: left to right; Hebrew: right to left). To provide some kind of a map for plotting phonic events, one must make available a structure that orders time. One such structure is rhythm; rhythm is organized time. Indeed, one reason why we tend to recall songs and poetry more efficiently than we do prose, even when there is no rhyming, is because of the overt cues–the orderliness–provided by the rhythm of the presentation. When there is also rhyme, the task becomes even easier; the regular pattern of salient acoustical attributes provides additional overt cues for organizing the sensations into meaningful sub-assemblies.

Thus, the rationale appears to be applicable to both visual and auditory perceptual skills development. To assess a child’s auditory-motor skills means scaling his performance along three dimensions. If his ability to receive and generate articulated speech is appropriately dif­ferentiated for his age, and if he displays no more need for overt self-produced cues (in terms of vocalizing what he has heard) and for environment cues (in terms of requiring a more orderly presentation and/or visual mediation) than do his age peers, he will be judged as having dem­onstrated “normal” auditory-motor skills. If his performance is less than satisfactory, the problem becomes one of determining where he is lacking–along which dimension(s) and to what degree.

General-Motor Skills Development

Although this topic was treated to some degree in the first section of the rationale, additional comments should now be made. General-motor skills refer to those gross and fine motor processes that appear to have some relationship with visual and auditory perception. These include such gross motor functions as balancing, hopping, and skipping, as well as the more refined behaviors called for in eye movements, vo­calizations, and digital manipulations.

As has been frequently stated in this paper, there are two primary sources of support available to the perceiver: the cues provided by the environment and those that he produces himself. These latter have been related to the tactile-kinesthetic information that is available to the visual perceiver by making physical contact with what he sees, and to the auditory perceiver by vocalizing what he hears. It is logical, then, that the more discrete these motor functions are, the more refined will be the sup­port derived through tactile-kinesthetic exploration of the sensory data.

There is yet another viable speculation. It has been suggested that the child learns to organize two-dimensional space by inferring a set of spatial coordinates upon that space–by looking at visual patterns as though through a map of vertical and horizontal coordinates. It has been proposed that as the map becomes more differentiated–as more coordi­nates are added to it–more refined spatial relationships can be plotted. There appears to be some evidence that a positive relationship exists between the differentiation of body scheme, as demonstrated by such be­haviors as hopping or balancing on one foot, the refinement of the map that the child infers on visual patterns, and the way in which certain per­formance tasks are solved (Witkin et al., 1962). The premise is that the more global the body scheme, the less differentiated the map of coordi­nates, and, thus, the less analytical the child’s psychomotor skills.

The same proposition can also be extended to three-dimensional visual space. A strong argument can be made for the hypothesis that man organizes visual space on an inferred map of coordinates representing the three dimensions of vertical, horizontal, and relative distance from self (Gesell, Ilg, & Bullis, 1949). Spatial localizations are relatively accurately made in a lighted, well-defined space in which objects are positioned in an orderly fashion (e.g., a furnished room). They are more difficult to perform in a less well-defined space that contains few objects (e.g., an open field). They are most difficult–in fact, impossible–to perform accurately in an undefined space, containing no objects, and devoid of gravity (e.g., outer space) (Howard & Templeton, 1966). These observations can also be related to the proposed rationale. A well-de­fined space and the objects contained within it function as topological cues; the nodes from which the three-dimensional coordinates may be inferred. As the cues are removed, the viewer must infer those also; he must act as though they were present in the environment. Finally, when the very basic cue provided by gravity is removed, the viewer no longer has any basis from which to infer support, insofar as spatial localizations beyond arm’s reach are concerned.

Is this pertinent to classroom performance? Undoubtedly! From his first day in school, the child is asked to shift his gaze between var­ious points in the classroom space–the chalkboard, the wall, his desk, the teacher (in any number of locations), and so forth. To do this effi­ciently, he must direct his eyes accurately and make spatial judgments (particularly left to right directional shifts) with little, if any, overt tactile-kinesthetic involvement. The accuracy with which a child directs his visual gaze, the way in which he is able to maintain fixation upon the pertinent targets, the efficiency with which he shifts visual attention from blackboard to desk to teacher–all oculomotor behaviors–are functions of the relative organization of his visual space. If he cannot organize that space, if he views it as an undefined mass containing a disordered array of an indeterminate number of objects, he will display inefficient oculomotor skills. If, on the other hand, he has the capacity to order the visual space of the classroom, if he can plot the relative positions of the various visual targets in that room (teacher, chalkboard, desk, etc.) on an inferred, refined three-dimensional map of coordinates, he is more likely to display parsimonious oculomotor behaviors.

The implication is that the inattentive, distractible child is often reacting to the overwhelming (to him) demands arising from attempting to control oculomotor functions in a disorganized space. Admittedly, there is such a phenomenon as impaired oculomotor function that is independent of the conditions of the task. But, the child who can show adequate oculo­motor behavior in one environment will predictably show it in any other that can be ordered to his level of need. (Although I will not elaborate upon it here, an operational space also contains an affective factor–and there is very good reason to suggest that a disordered “affective space” can also cause disruption of efficient processes. The child must develop the capacity to infer organization on relatively undefined psychodynamic spaces. Emotional maturity is, in effect, just that.)

An analogous relationship seems to exist between certain motor skills and auditory perception. As stated, a two- or three-dimensional map of coordinates cannot apply to the ordering of sequentially produced phonic events–events that occur over time. Temporal sequencing appears to be more closely linked to such rhythmic behaviors as skipping, rhythmic hopping (Kephart, 1960), and rhythmic tapping (Rosner et al., 1969), all of which require that the two body sides relate in synchronous fashion. Luria (1966) also suggests this relationship in his comments regarding auditory perception.

General-motor development, therefore, seems to be a pertinent factor in a discussion about perceptual skills, for at least two reasons. First, in that overt manipulation of visual constructs enhances the development of efficient visual analysis skills (Held & Hein, l963) and that overt manipulation (vocalization) of acoustical constructs is important to the development of efficient auditory analysis skills. The refinement of these two sets of manipulative skills, therefore, contributes to the establishment of those differentiated perceptual abilities that are assumed in a first-grade classroom for the “unimpaired.”

Secondly, it is suggested that the child’s ability to order visual space is directly related to the level of differentiation achieved in his own concept of body scheme; and, that his ability to order acoustical events is directly related to the degree to which he has learned to interrelate his two body sides in a rhythmic, synchronous fashion. This does not imply that properly developed motor skills guarantee satisfactory audi­tory and visual perceptual skills; only that a relationship does exist.

If these assumptions are supported by empirical research, it will be evidence that the child’s general-motor skills are important, not be­cause they relate directly to such classroom tasks as learning to read and calculate but, because they subserve critical, higher order perceptual skills that may, in turn, relate directly to classroom achieve­ment. More than general motor differentiation is needed for adequate visual and auditory perceptual skills, however. It is one factor–but not the only one. Awkward motor skills do not necessarily predict unsatis­factory academic achievement; nor on the other hand, do highly differen­tiated motor skills necessarily predict capable classroom performance. The general-motor skills seem to be important, but primarily only as they offer support to the higher order perceptual skills of vision and audition.

Summary

The rationale, as stated, proposes that the ability to analyze the visual and acoustical constructs of the communication codes is critical to satisfactory classroom achievement. It further postulates that these analytical skills are closely interrelated with motor functions –particu­larly the fine motor skills of prehension and vocalization. The pervasive theme of this rationale has been that these analysis and organization skills interrelate and become more refined over time (Global –> Differentiated) as well as more efficient (sensory-MOTOR –> SENSORY-motor), de­pending less on both overt environmental and self-produced cues. Syn­thesis, production behaviors, has been stressed as the appropriate avenue for determining the degree to which analytical skills have been acquired; discrimination tests fall short of the mark.

Concern for parsimonious function has also been emphasized. Analysis skills are essential–but not exclusively. Analysis skills en­able the child to disassemble a visual or acoustical construct–to sort out the coding elements and order them. Efficient classroom function, however, requires that analysis not continue to operate on this basic coding unit level; that as the child becomes familiar with the salient attributes of the classroom symbolic constructs, he will start to organize information at a higher level. That is, alphabet letters will not be ana­lyzed down to the “straight lines and circles” level; eventually, printed words will not be analyzed down to individual letters, and so on. The basic unit of analysis will have expanded from relatively small, segmen­ted bits to linked chains of behaviors–larger and fewer units. As such, just as with the student driver, the child will not need to devote energy to a myriad of separate actions; he will analyze constructs only to a level that is needed for efficient performance.

Goal 1 - Identify those perceptual skills that appear to be directly related to the basic classroom tasks of reading and arithmetic at the primary-grade level.

As stated in the rationale, the perceptual skills we sought to de­fine were those that represented the child’s ability to analyze visual and acoustical constructions according to specific attributes and to display these skills by productions rather than by recognition and discrimination responses. Our strategy was to: (1) identify currently available tests that probed what appeared to be critical perceptual skills; (2) administer these tests to a broad sample of preschool, kindergarten, and primary-grade children; (3) test this same population with appropriate standardized academic achievement batteries; (4) search for significant relationships between the children’s performances on the various perceptual skills tests and their achievement test scores; and (5) given evidence of a significant relationship between a specific perceptual skill and one or more of the academic achievement subtest scores, analyze that perceptual skill in an attempt to discover the relevant dimensions of the behavior involved; this, in an effort to construct a test or series of tests that could probe the child’s mastery of the basic underlying processes of that be­havior, rather than his ability to meet the demands of a selected test item.

For example, given evidence that linked copying skills with some aspect of first-grade achievement, we then would search for a way of assessing the child’s mastery of the generalizable processes required for copying designs, rather than concentrating on content–that is, on a specific geometric design or series of designs.

During the first year of the Project, the first four steps of the strategy were implemented. A number of perceptual tests were admin­istered to approximately 300 kindergarten and first-grade pupils, at the beginning and, again, at the end of the academic year; these data were then related to the same population’s scores on the Wide Range Achieve­ment Test. The perceptual tests included two standardized visual-motor instruments: the Gesell Copy Forms (Ilg & Ames, 1964) and the Rutgers Drawing Test (Starr, 1961), and two auditory perception instruments: the Word Repetition Test (Rosner et al., 1969)–a shortened version of the Slingerland Echolalia subtest–and the Auditory Organization Test (Rosner, 1966). A number of other tests were tried and abandoned for a variety of reasons; the primary reason being those tests’ inadequacies in discrim­inating well among children of this age, either because their criteria were too easy or too demanding.

Analysis of the data derived from this approach yielded some en­couraging results. Some significant correlations were derived with indications that the auditory tests we used tended to predict reading achievement better than arithmetic achievement, and that the two stan­dardized visual-motor tests appeared to be more closely related to arith­metic than to reading (Rosner & Cooley, 1971). Using these data, an attempt was made to determine the extent to which changes in perceptual skills affected end-of-year achievement in arithmetic and reading. Mul­tiple correlations for the full set of predictors (i.e., beginning-of-year and end-of-year Scores) were compared to multiple correlations using only the beginning-of-year scores. The differences between the multiple correlations using the first set of predictors and those using the total set of predictors reveal the amount of information, regarding achieve­ment, in the second group of measures that was not in the initial set. The results of that analysis were consistent with the proposition that changes in perceptual skills, over the course of the school year, affect end-of-year achievement in both reading and arithmetic; that is, there is information in spring perceptual skills that is not in fall perceptual skills regarding end-of-year achievement.

Visual-Motor Skills

The next step, along with continued testing to replicate the above findings and to search for other pertinent predictors, was an attempt to discover the relevant dimensions of those perceptual skills that had been shown to meet the criteria of Goal 1. For example, it obviously was not the ability to copy the specific geometric designs of the two tests–the Gesell Copy Forms and the Rutgers Drawing Test–that was important. If Goal 2 was to be achieved–if we were to be successful in teaching the skills identified as pertinent–it was necessary to define the processes that applied to copying in general. Attending to the assumptions of the rationale, a series of studies was initiated to discover and describe a set of visual-motor tasks that would ask the following questions along a scale of increasing complexity: (1) Can the child demonstrate the ability to “see” a geometric design as a finite number of elements by reproduc­ing those same elements? (2) Can the child demonstrate the ability to see” the interrelationships between these elements, when they are pre­sented on a map of coordinates? And (3) can the child demonstrate the ability to infer a map of coordinates by reproducing a pattern, presented on a map of spatial coordinates, in a space where no map, or only a partial map, is provided?

Figure 7 illustrates the tasks that were finally designed, following a series of pilot studies: a 27-item battery, identified as the Visual Analysis Test (VAT: Rosner, 1971c). All of the first 18 items are ad­ministered in the same way. The testee is asked to “Make this side (Examiner points to response space on right side) look just like this side (Examiner points to stimulus pattern on left side). Draw the lines on this side (Examiner points) so that it looks just like this side (Examiner points).” For items 19 through 27, the Examiner adds, “Some of the dots are missing on this side (Examiner points to right side). Don’t draw the dots, just draw the lines as though the dots were there. Imagine (or pretend) that the dots are there.” (For item 27, Examiner says: “All of the dots are missing…etc.”)

Figure 7

Figure7

VAT Items (reduced to less than 1/6 actual size)

Inspection of the test items shown in Figure 7 reveals the increas­ing complexity of the tasks along the three dimensions defined above:

  1. The number of elements in the design;
  2. The intricacies of their interre­lationships and of the map on which the geometric design is shown; and,
  3. The number of overt spatial cues provided in the space where the response is to be drawn.

It seemed reasonable to postulate that if these were the relevant dimensions of copying skills in general, then scaling higher along these three variables should predict higher scores on a standardized copying test. Given evidence to support that hypothesis, it would then be possi­ble, using items from the VAT, to define an organized sequence of instruc­tional objectives for teaching the visual-motor analysis skills that underlie copying. That, of course, was the purpose of constructing the VAT. There is no need for another standardized copying test; our goal was not to produce a standardized test. Rather, we were searching for a way of describing and scaling the general processes of visual-motor development in behavioral terms.

To score the test, a transparency defining arbitrarily determined limits was prepared for each item; the transparency is superimposed over the child’s response. The scorer then applies a relatively simple set of criteria to score each item on a three-point scale (0, 1, 2) that is based upon how closely the child’s responses resemble the model. Possible scores range from 0 to 54. Minimum interrater reliability (Pearson Product Moment Correlation coefficients were calculated be­tween three scorers, working independently on 100 randomly selected tests) in scoring the VAT was demonstrated to be .98; hence, the scoring procedure proved to be highly dependable.

The test was administered to 667 kindergarten, first-, and second-grade children, the total enrollment (of those grades) in three suburban Pittsburgh schools. None of these children had been tested before by this Project. Table 1 shows the means, standard deviations, medians, and ranges for each of the grade levels in each of the three schools. These data show a broad range of scores both within and across grades; they also show a direct relationship between the VAT score and chrono­logical age, as indicated by grade level.

The next question was: do the behaviors tested by the VAT have any relationship to those sampled in a standardized copying test? To answer this, the 667 kindergarten, first-, and second-grade children referred to in Table 1 had also been tested with both the Rutgers Drawing Test as well as the Visual Analysis Test, in that order. Pearson Product Moment Correlations between the two test scores were .80 with the kindergarten and first-grade groups and .68 with the second-grade group; both coefficients are significant well above the .01 level of con­fidence. Clearly, relatively valid predictions concerning a child’s performance on the Rutgers Drawing Test could he made from his respon­ses to the VAT.

1. Form A of the Rutgers Drawing Test was used in the kindergarten and first grade. Form B was used in the second grade.

Table 1

VAT Mean, Median, and Range of Scores for

Kindergarten, Grades 1 and 2

Grade School N VAT S.D. Median

Mean

Range
A 94 20.6 9.3 20 2-47
K B*
C 161 16.2 8.7 16 0-42
A 83 33.4 7.7 34 15-48
1 B 59 33.5 8.8 36 10-48
C 73 31.0 9.9 32 7-48
A 74 41.2 6.1 42 24-52
2 B 45 38.2 6.5 38 23-49
C 78 34.3 8.2 35 13-49

* – This school does not sponsor a kindergarten class.

These outcomes support but do not confirm the basic objectives of Goal 1; that is, to identify perceptual skills that are directly related to primary-grade reading and arithmetic achievement. All that could be said from the study just described was that the VAT appeared to have much in common with the standardized Rutgers Drawing Test and that the former could be scored with a great deal more reliability than can most copying tests.

Yet to be answered were two critical questions: (1) Is the VAT directly related to the reading and/or arithmetic achievement of primary-grade children? And (2) if the first question is answered affirmatively, does the VAT predict achievement to at least the same degree as a well-established, standardized copying test? That is, can as much of the variance in academic achievement be accounted for by the VAT as by the Rutgers Drawing Test?

The following studies were conducted in an attempt to answer those two questions. The VAT and Rutgers Drawing Test scores of 154 first- and 153 second-graders (the total student population in those grades in two suburban Pittsburgh public schools) were related to this same popu­lation’s Stanford Achievement Test scores in the subtests of Word Read­ing and Arithmetic Computations. Table 2 shows these correlation co­efficients and the outcomes of the tests to determine the significance between these experimentally dependent correlations.

Table 2

Coefficients of Correlation between VAT, Rutgers Drawing Test, and the Word Reading and Arithmetic Subtests of Stanford Achievement Test

Grade 1 (N 154) Grade 2 (N l53)
Word Rdg. Arith. *p Word Rdg. Arith. *p
VAT .40 .55 <.025 36 .58 <.005
Rutgers .34 .35 NS .40 .46 NS
*p NS <.0005 X NS <.025 X

NS = not significant

*p = significance between experimentally dependent correlations

Notice that, while there is not much difference between the ways in which the Rutgers relates to the Word Reading and Arithmetic subtests, the VAT appears to be more closely related to the Arithmetic achieve­ment scores than to Word Reading. For example, in grade 1, the Rut­gers accounts for approximately 12 percent of the variance in both of the achievement subtests, while the VAT accounts for over 30 percent of the variance in the Arithmetic subtest scores in contrast to only 16 percent of the variance in Word Reading. There obviously is no signi­ficant difference between the two first-grade correlation coefficients that involve the Rutgers; on the other hand, the difference between the two correlation coefficients involving the VAT is different at a signifi­cance level of .025 (t = 2.265; df = 151).2 Similar findings are shown in grade 2 also. Again, there is no significant difference between the two correlation coefficients involving the Rutgers (.40 and .46), while the difference between the two that include the VAT (.36 and .58) is different at a .005 level of significance (t = 2.81; df = 150). 3

It is also important to note that, while both the Rutgers and the VAT appear to predict Arithmetic scores more closely than they pre­dict Word Reading achievement scores, the VAT clearly outperforms the Rutgers. The latter, in grade 1, accounts for 12 percent of the variance in the Arithmetic achievement subtest as compared to the 30 percent accounted for by the VAT. This difference is significant at a .0005 level (t = 3.764; df =151).4 The same phenomenon, albeit to a lesser degree, is apparent in grade 2, where the difference between the two correlation coefficients (VAT and Rutgers), as they relate to Arith­metic, is significant at the .025 level (t = 2.273; df = 150).5

2. Word Reading. Arithmetic (grade 1) r = .519

3. Word Reading. Arithmetic (grade 2) r = .418

4. VAT.Rutgers (grade 1) r .696

5. VAT. Rutgers (grade 2) r = .686

As stated, these analyses were conducted to answer two questions concerning the VAT: namely, its capacity to predict primary-grade reading and/or arithmetic achievement and, given some positive indica­tions regarding the test’s predictive validity, its relationship to achieve­ment as contrasted with a well-established, standardized copying test. The answer to the first question is apparent–the VAT is closely related with primary-grade arithmetic achievement, significantly moreso than with reading achievement, thus reaffirming the data reported earlier (Rosner & Cooley, 1971). More noteworthy, the VAT not only displays a close relationship to a standardized copying test; it also was shown to be a significantly better predictor of arithmetic achievement than was the Rutgers. Neither test outperformed the other in predicting reading achievement. These outcomes indicate that the VAT approximates Goal 1 objectives.

Auditory-Motor Skills

A similar approach was taken to identify those auditory perceptual skills that were pertinent to the goals of the Project; i.e. , search for be­haviors that were representative of auditory skills in general as well as being related to some aspect of academic achievement.

Referring once more to the Rosner and Cooley (1971) study, the auditory skills tested in that initial battery (Auditory Organization; Word Repetition) tended to show closer relationships to reading than to arith­metic. The next step here, then, was to discover and describe behaviorally the underlying processes pertinent to auditory perception so that we could design a method for testing them. Undoubtedly, it was not simply the ability to repeat the kinds of confusable words that appear in the Word Repetition Test nor the clapping patterns of the Auditory Organization Test. These obviously were two ways to sample for the presence of the skills listed in the rationale: namely, the child’s ability to demonstrate that he recognizes spoken words as constructed of a series of finite phonic events that interrelate or occur in a specific sequence. They did not, however, isolate the basic processes we were seeking to define. Further efforts were required.

By our definition of perceptual skills, discrimination tests were ruled out a priori. Embedded sound tests were tried in pilot studies and rejected because of their failure to discriminate well among the age group in whom we were interested. They were too difficult. A test was needed that could ask the following questions along a scale of increasing complexity:

  1. Can the child demonstrate the ability to “hear” a spoken word as a finite number of blended phonic elements?
  2. Can the child demonstrate the ability to “hear” the interrelationships of those elements in some production behavior? and,
  3. Can the child show these two abilities using increasingly complex verbal constructs?

Eventually, after examin­ing many published instruments and after numerous trial and error attempts at developing our own, we constructed a test that appeared to ask the correct questions. The test contained 40 items of varying difficulty; it was identified as the Auditory Analysis Test (AAT: Rosner & Simon,1971).6 The AAT contains such items as “Say birthday. Now say it again, but don’t say birth.”; “Say man. Now say it again, but don’t say /m/ [the ''m'' sound]. ”Say gate. Now say it again, but don’t say /t/ [the t sound].” A sample of the test recording sheet is shown in Figure 8 The test is administered, as indicated above, by first asking the testee to say the item word; after he has repeated it, the Examiner asks him to “Say it again, but don’t say ____ ,” indicating the specific sound to be omitted; (the sound(s) to be omitted are those that are shown in parentheses on the test recording sheet). Letter names are not used; rather, only the sounds to be deleted. Items A and B are introductory words to be used to pro­vide instruction about the test. Testing stops after four consecutive errors; each item is scored either correct or incorrect. Total possible scores, therefore, range from 0 to 40.

Inspection of the test items shown in Figure 8, in the order in which they were administered, reveals the increasing complexity of the task along the three dimensions defined above:

  1. The number and size of the phonic elements (e.g. , from syllables to single phonemes);
  2. The relative position of the phonic element in the word (e.g. , from a beginning, to a final, to a medially positioned sound); and
  3. The complexity of the verbal construct in which the sound is embedded (e.g., from one syllable, to multisyllable words, to an isolated phoneme followed and/or preceded by a vowel, to part of a consonant blend).

Our thinking in con­structing the AAT was that if these really reflected the relevant dimen­sions of auditory perception skills in general, then scaling higher along the three variables should predict better performance in other tasks that required analytical listening skills. Given evidence to support that no­tion, it would then be possible to define an organized sequence of instruc­tional objectives, based on the items of the AAT, for teaching auditory analysis skills. That, of course, was one of our major objectives. We did not seek to develop another standardized auditory perception test. Rather, we were looking for a way to describe and scale the underlying skills of auditory-motor development in behavioral terms.

In the initial study, 234 first- through sixth-grade children (the total population of a suburban Pittsburgh public school) were tested with the AAT. Table 3 summarizes the test scores for the six grade levels. The data indicate that the demands of the AAT are met more readily by children as they mature and progress through an academic program, al­though marked individual differences are also apparent within each grade level.

6 Shortly after this study had been released, an earlier study (Bruce, 1964), concerned with remarkably similar behaviors, came to my attention. Bruce had confined his study to five- through seven-year-old children, and his items were less diverse and administered differently. He did not relate his subjects’ responses to reading ability. His conclusion was that the ability to analyze word sounds is a function of maturation and that a “mental age of 7+ is indicated as the level at which the skills become available in sufficient degree to permit some success with the task (p. 158).”

Auditory Analysis Test

Name_________________________________ Date_______________________

School______________________ Grade____ Teacher__________Birth Date____________

A. cow(boy)

B. (tooth)brush

1. birth(day) 2. (car)pet 3. bel(t) 4. (m)an
5. (b)lock 6. to(ne) 7. (s)our 8. (p)ray
9. stea(k) 10. (l)end 11. (s)mile 12. plea(se)
13. (g)ate 14. (c)lip 15. ti(me) 16. (sc)old
17. (b)reak 18. ro(de) 19. (w)ill 20. (t)rail
21. (sh)rug 22. g(l)ow 23. cr(e)ate 24. (st)rain
25. s(m)ell 26. Es(ki)mo 27. de(s)k 28. Ger(ma)ny
29. st(r)eam 30. auto(mo)bile 31. re(pro)duce 32. s(m)ack
33. phi(lo)sophy 34. s(k)in 35. lo(ca)tion 36. cont(in)ent
37. s(w)ing 38. car(pen)ter 39. c(l)utter 40. off(er)ing

Figure 8

Figure8

Sample Test Recording Sheet

Table 3

Auditory Analysis Test Mean Scores by Grade

Grade N Mean S.D. Median Range
1 53 17.6 8.4 17.6 2-35
2 41 19.9 9.3 17.6 1-36
3 37 25.1 8.5 25.5 6-37
4 29 25.7 7.9 28.7 9-35
5 35 28.1 7.6 30.8 11-38
6 39 29.9 6.9 32.3 15-38

During the month in which this testing was conducted (April, 1970), the same children were also given the Stanford Achievement Test. For purposes of validation, the AAT scores were then correlated with the Language Arts subtests of that achievement test.7 These coefficients are shown in Table 4.

That a close relationship exists between the behaviors measured by the AAT and the Stanford Language Arts subtests is apparent. The curvilinear nature of that relationship is interesting. One can speculate that this may be a function of the achievement subtests, in that their first-grade items tend to require less phonetic decoding skills than do the second-grade items which, in turn, are not as demanding as the third-grade items. The fourth-, fifth-, and sixth-grade achievement subtest items, on the other hand, appear to sample comprehension skills to a greater extent. They are not as closely related to basic de­coding abilities and, hence, to the phonic analysis skills that are assessed by the AAT. Further and more detailed study is necessary, however, before this reasoning can be viewed as something more than intuition.

Table 4

Mean Auditory Analysis Test Scores and Correlations with Language Arts Skills

Mean Correlation Coefficients
Grade N AAT Scores S.D. AAT – LA
1 53 17.6 8.4 .53
2 41 19.9 9.3 .62
3 37 25.1 8.5 .84
4 29 25.7 7.9 .72
5 35 28.1 7.6 .75
6 39 29.9 6.9 .59

Replication studies have been conducted since that initial effort. The data, as yet unpublished, are consistent with our initial discoveries in showing a very strong relationship between auditory skills and reading achievement.

We then compared the relationships of the two perceptual tests, the VAT and the AAT, to arithmetic as well as to reading achievement. To do this the AAT was compared to the VAT, insofar as how both re­lated to the Word Reading and Arithmetic subtests of the Stanford Achieve­ment Test The population of this study was all of the first- and second-­grade children in three suburban Pittsburgh

7 Language Arts subtest scores were treated as a single combined score by totaling the stanines in the five subtests of Word Meaning, Paragraph Reading, Vocabulary, Spelling, and Word Study Skills.

schools. These correlation coefficients, and the outcomes of the tests to determine the significance between these experimentally dependent correlations, are shown in Table 5.

Table 5

Coefficients of Correlation between the AAT, VAT, and the Word Reading

and Arithmetic Subtests of the Stanford Achievement Test

Grade 1 (N = 215) Grade 2 (N = 219)
Word Reading Arithmetic Word Reading Arithmetic
AAT .53 .39 .49 .34
VAT .39 .57 .25 .50
*p <. 025 <.005 <.005 <.01

*p = significance between experimentally dependent correlations

Here again we have evidence of the closer relationship between arithmetic and the visual-motor skills of copying, and between reading and a set of auditory-motor behaviors. Notice in grade 1, for example, that the AAT accounts for 28 percent of the variance in Word Reading, while the VAT accounts for only 15 percent of the variance in that same achievement subtest. This difference is significant at the .025 level (t = 2.215; df = 212).8 In contrast, the AAT accounts for 15 percent of the variance in the Arithmetic subtest, while the VAT accounts for some­what over 32 percent of the variance in that same subtest. These two differ at the .005 level of significance (t = 2.747; df = 215). 8

These specific relationships between perceptual behaviors and the two academic domains are evident in the second-grade data as well. Once again the AAT relates more closely to the Word Reading subtest, account­ing for 24 percent of the variance as compared to the 6 percent that is accounted for by the VAT. These two are significantly different at the .005 level (t = 2.924; df = 216).9 The last comparison to be discussed is the way in which the VAT and the AAT relate to the Arithmetic subtest of this second-grade population. The difference once more is statistically significant (.01) (t = 2.371; df = 216), the VAT accounting for 25 percent of the variance in that subtest; the AAT accounting for 11 percent.

These outcomes, then, also supported our efforts toward the accomplishment of Goal 1 in that the skills tested by the AAT are not only directly concerned with auditory perception but are also specifically re­lated to achievement in the domain of primary-grade language arts.

8 AAT – VAT (grade 1) r = .35

Summary

It should again be stressed that in no sense was the VAT nor the AAT intended to serve the functions of standardized tests that are based on representative normative data. Rather, in both cases, the tests were constructed for two other purposes. First, in the form they were de­signed, they could be administered and scored easily, providing relative­ly inexpensive access to the information which was essential to our goal–namely, the identification of perceptual behaviors that appeared to re­late to academic achievement.

The second purpose, one that will be elaborated upon in the sec­tion of this paper devoted to Goal 2, was also vital to our needs. That is, if it could be demonstrated that the behaviors sampled by the AAT and VAT were worth measuring, our next quest was to determine if such per­ceptual skills could be trained. An examination of the VAT and AAT test items will quickly reveal that they range from those that are apparently easy, to some that are quite complex; that is, from global to differentiated. To train such skills, it would be useful to know how they scaled; which were easiest, which were most difficult, and so forth. Given that informa­tion, effective training sequences could be constructed.

The data from the initial studies of the AAT and VAT, as discussed above, provided that kind of information. By testing a large number of different aged children, and by analyzing the item responses, we could scale the relative difficulty of each item, identify the general common traits of those that appeared to he of similar difficulty, and construct a series of training procedures in which the objectives of the training were sequenced in some valid manner. This, then, was the second purpose for constructing and using the VAT and AAT.

General-Motor Skills

To date, we have not researched sufficiently the relationship between general-motor skills and academic achievement; studies are currently underway, but little of substance can be offered at this time. Our initial observations, however tend to indicate that a direct relationship between general-motor skills and academic achievement, independent of visual and auditory perceptual abilities, cannot be supported with any real vigor. What relationships appear to exist seem to link certain motor skills with visual and auditory perceptual skills in a hierarchical manner, in which the motor skills appear to serve as subordinate factors to the relatively higher order functions of auditory and visual analysis which, in turn, pro­vide some of the basic information processing skills assumed by reading and arithmetic instructional programs.

Goal 1 has been attained, at least to some degree. Certainly there may be other critical perceptual skills that have not yet been identi­fied. One area of promise, for example, appears to be in the application of time limitations on visual-motor functions. Pilot studies have been conducted, using VAT-like items

9. AAT.VAT (grade 2) r = .29

that are to be completed within a fixed amount of time. The initial data are sufficiently encouraging to stimulate further investigation in this direction.

We will continue to search for pertinent skills; but, enough progress has been made toward this goal to justify the initiation of efforts towards Goal 2.

Goal 2 – Given identified perceptual skills that do relate directly to classroom achievement, determine whether such skills can he trained effectively.

The information reported in the last section appeared to justify pursuance of Goal 2, while still devoting efforts to adding to the data that are relevant to Goal 1. Highly significant relationships had been demon­strated between the copying skills sampled in the VAT and arithmetic, and between the phonic analysis skills sampled in the AAT and reading. Lesser, though still significant, relationships were demonstrated be­tween VAT scores and reading and between AAT scores and arithmetic. The next question appeared to be: Can such perceptual skills be trained or are they solely dependent upon such factors as growth, development, and genetic pre-determination–are they or are they not modifiable?

Visual-Motor Skills

A study was conducted with a group of four-year-olds (range: 3.9 to 4.9 years; median: 4.4), testing that question. As a pretest (Time 1) the Rutgers Drawing Test, Form A (Starr, 1961) was administered to the total preschool population (N 29) in the two classrooms of a local urban school that is participating in the Primary Education Project, an LRDC early learning program (Resnick, 1967). The Design Board Pro­gram, a sub-component of the Visual-motor portion of the Perceptual Skills Curriculum, was then presented to all of the children (N 14) in one of the two classrooms (Class A). (The Design Board Program is in­tended to teach the processes involved in copying skills that were con­sidered in constructing the VAT. It does not focus on specific designs; rather it is aimed at teaching generalizable skills. Many of its objectives resemble very closely the test items of the VAT. Some description of this program will be given in the Goal 4 section of this paper; for more details, see Rosner, 1971a.)

Training was individually administered by a Research Assistant to Class A, with sessions lasting approximately ten minutes. The chil­dren in Class B participated in their regular daily programs; they were given no special substitute treatment. Four or five children were trained each day for a period of 47 days. Their accumulated training times ranged from 5 to 16 sessions, depending upon the regularity of their attendance, with the average being 12.7 (median 13) days. The Rutgers Drawing Test (Form A) was again administered (Time 2) to both classes (A and B) following that training period. Similar training, under the supervision of a second Research Assistant, was then introduced into the second class (Class B), following the same format of individually admin­istered 10 minute training sessions. The accumulated training time of the children in this group varied from 7 to 17 sessions, averaging 12.5 (median 13) days. A third Rutgers Drawing Test was administered (Time 3) following the completion of the training of Class B.

Figure 9 represents the class Rutgers Drawing Test mean scores at Times 1, 2, and 3. At Time 1, no statistical difference is shown between the scores of Class A and Class B. Following training, at Time 2, the scores of Class A are different from B, at a .001 level of significance, using an analysis of covariance statistic with the Time 1 Rutgers scores of the two groups serving as covariates. At Time 3, after Class B had also been trained, statistical differences between groups no longer exist. Using the normative data provided by the Rutgers Drawing Test, the Mean Equivalent Drawing Age of the two classes, at Time 1, was less than 48 months. Their Mean Equivalent Drawing Age at Time 3 was 58 months. The actual time that elapsed between Tirne 1 and Time 3 was 4 months.

Figure 9

Figure9

Rutgers Drawing Test Mean Scores for Classes A and B at Times I, 2, and 3

The mean Rutgers Drawing Test scores of Classes A and B com­bined, at Time 3, were then compared to those of the kindergarten children in the same school. No statistical significance was shown between the Rutgers Drawing Test scores of this combined group of four-year-old nursery school children and a kindergarten group of children of similar background, enrolled in the same school, whose mean age was 10 months greater. It appears then, at least with these two groups, that the visual-motor skills of copying were amenable to training.

Another study of similar nature was initiated with two classes (C and D) of three-year-olds in the same school. Initially, no significant differences were shown between the Rutgers Drawing Test scores of the two groups. Their Rutgers Drawing Test scores were again compared after the children of Class C had received visual-motor training. This time, because of the small groups involved (C: N 5; D: N = 6), the Mann-Whitney U test was used. Class C’s copying scores, after train­ing, were significantly higher (p .041) than Class D’s.

In yet another study, a multiple baseline design (Revusky, 1967) was used to explore, among other questions, the effects of visual-motor training on the copying skills of children who were not attending an LRDC developmental school.10 Through the cooperation of the Research Depart­ment of the Pittsburgh Board of Public Education, and the Principal of one of its schools, such a kindergarten group was made available.

Among other pretests (that will be referred to in the Goal 3 section), ten children were given the Geometric Design subtest of the Performance Scale of WPPSI (Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence, 1967). One child, chosen at random from the ten, was then provided with visual-motor training, using the Design Board Program. When this first child had achieved a predesignated objective in the program, he and the other nine children in the original sample were retested with the Geometric Design subtest. Following that testing, a second child was chosen at ran­dom, trained to criterion, and retested along with the remainder of the untrained group. The scheme was followed until five children were trained; average training time was 20 minutes per day for 22 days. The Revusky research design is based on the notion of treating each training segment as a sub-experiment. The test results of all the subjects, at the end of each sub-experiment, are ranked, yielding an Rn statistic. To achieve significance at the .01 level, with a population of this size, the subject who has just received the training must rank first among all of the subjects in each sub-experiment. This was the case. Each time the group was retested with the Geometric Design subtest of the WPPSI (at the end of each sub-experiment), it was the child who had just been trained who showed the greater positive change in copying skills.

10 The study in which this information is included was reported at the AERA Annual Meeting, New York, 1971 (Rosner, Levine, & Simon, 1971).

The data were also analyzed a second way. By the end of the school year, as stated, five children had been trained; five had not. An analysis of covariance was conducted, using the pretest Geometric De­sign subtest scores as covariates. The difference between the mean

scores of the trained and non-trained groups was again significant at the .05 level. The study extended over a six-month period. It is noteworthy that the mean scaled score of the non-trained group of children was exactly the same at the end of that six-month period as it had been at the pretest, (X = 10.6; S.D. = 2.6) while the mean scaled score of the trained group changed from 10.8(SD = 2.4) to 13.0(SD = 1.7). (Raw scores did change; scaled scores, of course, take into consideration the chronological age of the subject as well as accuracy of his performance.)

These three studies, dealing with children of ages 3, 4, and 5, respectively, all yielded the same type of outcome. It seems evident that visual-motor skills can be trained and that the effects of such train­ing are demonstrable in copying tasks where the test items are not at all identical to any used in the training procedures. In other words, the process of how the task is accomplished–how to copy geometric designs rather than a specific design–is being trained.

Auditory-Motor Skills

The question of Goal 2 was directed, as well, to auditory percep­tual skills. That is, can a child’s auditory perceptual skills be trained to a more discrete level or is improvement of these behaviors exclusive­ly dependent upon maturation? To address this question, a study was conducted at a suburban Pittsburgh school (Rosner, 1971b). At the be­ginning of the academic year it was determined that only sixteen of the forty entering first-graders were non-readers. All of the others had been taught at least some beginning reading skills while still in kinder­garten (see Beck & Mitroff, 1972, for a description of that beginning reading program).

The AAT was administered to the entire class (N = 40) at the be­ginning of their first-grade year (Time 1) and the scores of the readers (R) and non-readers (NR) were compared. The R group’s AAT scores were significantly higher (p < .01) than the NR’s scores. The children in the NR group (N = 16) were stratified into three subgroups according to their IQ (High, Middle, Low). Each of these three groups was then randomly divided into two subgroups, resulting in two populations of rela­tively equal IQ and AAT scores. One of these populations (N = 8 ) was designated as a Control group (C); the other (N = 8), as an Experimental group (E). Half of each group was placed with one first-grade teacher, the other half with another first-grade teacher. Hence, the E group and the C group were equally distributed between two teachers–this is an attempt to control for the teacher variable. Group E was given daily 15-minute auditory analysis training sessions, outside of the classroom, by a paraprofessional. (Some description of the training will be given in the Goal 4 section of this paper. The intent of the training is to teach the same processes that were considered in constructing the AAT. It does not focus on specific sounds or sequences of sounds. Many of its objectives resemble very closely the test items of the AAT.) Group C was given no substitute treatment. Both groups were given reading instruction in their respective classrooms along with the rest of their classmates. After 37 training sessions, Group E had all achieved a predetermined level of competency. The AAT was then re-administered to all 40 first-grade children (Time 2) and the scores of the untrained readers (R), the trained E group, and the untrained C group were compared, using an analysis of covariance with the AAT pretraining scores serving as the covariates. Table 6 shows the mean AAT scores of the three groups (R, E, and C) at Times 1 and 2 as well as their mean chronological ages and IQ’s.

Table 6

Mean Age, IQ, and Measure 1 and 2 AAT Scores

for Groups R, E, and C

Whereas the differences in AAT scores of groups E and C were not statistically significant in the pretest (prior to training), the post­training scores of the two groups were different at a .005 level of significance (F = 14.33; df = 1/13). Group R’s Time 2 scores also were compared to both groups E and C. The differences between R and E scores were no longer very marked (p < .10). On the other hand, the differences between R and C were now quite different (p<.0005), in favor of the R group. The changes shown by group R are worth commenting on. Al­though they were not given training specifically in auditory perception, their acquisition of skills is evident; group C, on the contrary, was not trained and made little gain. Why? A reasonable assumption is that, since group R could already read when first grade started, their continued reading instruction probably provided excellent training in auditory skills in that reading, at the primary–decoding–level, is essentially the pro­cess of learning that sounds can be mediated visually; reading is the re­constituting of those visual symbols back into speech.

The study was replicated with a different group of sixteen children about one month after the one described above had been completed. The outcomes were much the same as they were in the first study. The differ­ence between groups, in posttraining responses to the AAT, was signifi­cant at the .005 level, based on analysis of covariance with the pretest measures of both groups being treated as covariates. Thus, both audi­tory analysis training studies provided strong evidence that auditory per­ceptual skills can be acquired through instruction.

General-Motor Skills

The question has not yet been formally put to the general-motor skills. It seems reasonable, however, to argue that there already is a great deal of information available from the fields of physical education and physical therapy to show that such gross motor behaviors as balancing on one foot, hopping, and skipping, and such refined skills as oculomotor control and finger dexterity are highly trainable. Similar evidence exists about voco-motor controls; surely the presence of speech therapists in so many elementary schools and speech clinics is justified because of the modifiability of speech articulation abilities.

Goals 1 and 2, then, appear to have been attained ? at least to a sufficient degree to ask the questions posed in Goal 3.

Goal 3 – Given trainable perceptual skills that are relevant to classroom achievement, determine whether the effect of that training can be measured in classroom behaviors. In other words, can transfer be effected?

The last section offered evidence concerning the plasticity of per­ceptual skills–at least, those that we had identified and described in the items of the VAT and AAT. It seems apparent that these behaviors can be trained. The major question, now, is whether the effect of such train­ing can also affect the classroom behaviors that were related to visual and auditory perceptual skills.

Visual-Motor Training

Thus far, no study has yet been conducted by this Project that attempts to assess the direct impact of visual-motor training on school achievement. Prior to the beginning of this Project, a study was con­ducted under the sponsorship of the Mental Health Services Division of the Pittsburgh Public Schools (Richman, Rosner, & Scott, 1969). That study followed up on an earlier one (Rosner, Richman, & Scott, 1969) in which it had been demonstrated that children in public school classes for the emotionally disturbed demonstrated a significantly higher incidence of perceptual dysfunction than did children of the same age, sex, and IQ who were attending regular classes in the same buildings. In fact, inci­dence of perceptual dysfunction in the classes for emotionally disturbed (70 percent) more closely approximated the incidence found in classes for the educable mentally retarded (90 percent). In contrast, the incidence among the children in regular classes was 13 percent.

The follow-up study was designed to investigate the impact of per­ceptual training upon the academic performance of children with percep­tual dysfunction in these three types of classes: regular, emotionally disturbed, and educable mentally retarded. Another component of the study was intended to test the feasibility of using paraprofessional classroom aides to serve as the trainers.

The children ranged in age from 5 years, 10 months to 13 years, 4 months, with a ratio of three boys to each girl. IQ scores ranged from 62 to 121. Attrition reduced the original population of 175 (training N 99; control = 76) to 143 (training N = 94; control = 49) by the end of the study. The training groups were located in five classrooms in five different school buildings. The control group classes were distributed similarly.

Two different training approaches were used: The Frostig (1964) and the Pace (1968) programs. Thirty minutes of training was provided daily by the paraprofessional aides to small groups of six or seven chil­dren for a total of 66 days. Both of these programs emphasize visual-motor training; very little effort was devoted to auditory training.

All of the children were tested with the Wide Range Achievement Test (Jastak, Bijou, & Jastak, 1965) both before and after the training period; the differences between control and trained groups were compared with analysis of covariance, using the pretest scores as the covariates.

Improvement in some aspects of academic achievement, following training, was shown in all three trained groups. In the regular classes, the only WRAT subtest score that showed significant improvement after training, in contrast to the controls, was Arithmetic (p .05). Among the mentally retarded and the emotionally disturbed, significant improve­ment ( <.01 to < .05) was shown in all three subtests (Reading, Spelling, Arithmetic) of the WRAT. This study appears to support the premise that visual-motor training (because, in fact, both training programs are almost exclusively devoted to visual-motor skills) can have impact upon academic achievement. The fact that only the Arithmetic subtest scores improved in the regular classes is yet another indication of the relatively closer relationship between visual-motor skills and arithmetic achieve­ment as compared to reading.

This Project’s only effort in this same area was part of a pre­viously cited study in which an attempt was made to study the effects of visual-motor training on a group of psychomotor behaviors; behaviors that have been identified as having some validity in predicting school achievement, in that they are subtests of a standardized test of intelli­gence. I refer to the multiple baseline study mentioned in the previous section (Rosner, Levine, & Simon, 1971). Along with measuring the impact of visual-motor training on the Geometric Design subtest scores of kindergarten children, comparisons were also made in the other four subtest scores of the WPPSI performance scale. The differences between posttraining WPPSI performance scale scores of control and trained groups, using pretraining performance scale scores as covariates, were significant at the . OS level. Figure 10 illustrates the differences in pre­- and posttraining scores.

Admittedly, increases in the performance scale subtest scores of a standardized IQ test are not the same, nor even the equivalent, of im­provement in academic performance. It does, however, prompt specu­lation that whatever training effected these changes will probably have similar impact on school achievement. The hypothesis has yet to be tested to a sufficient degree. Additional studies, designed to address this question, are currently being conducted and will be reported as data are available.

Figure 10

Figure10

Mean Performance Scale Score of Experimental and Control Groups at Measures 1 and 5

Auditory-Motor Training

This problem was investigated in some of the studies cited in the previous section. Both studies that were concerned with the effects of training on auditory perceptual skills, reported in the last section, also contained a second question; that is, does auditory training have impact on the children’s reading skills as well as on their listening skills? In the initial study (Rosner, 1971b), both the E group and the C group were non-readers at the beginning of the school year. Both groups received reading instruction from their classroom teachers. Since both groups were distributed equally between two

The division of letters into six units was not as arbitrary as initial inspection would suggest. It is evident that lower case letters are more susceptible to directional confusions than are capital letters. For this reason, the lower case alphabet was divided into three basic classes:

  1. Those that, in all but one case, can be related to the lower case c; and, in addition, if they contain a vertical line, it appears to the right of the rest of the letter. The letters of Units 8 and 9 belong in this group.
  2. Those that, in all but three cases, can be related to the lower case r; and, in addition, if they contain a vertical line, it appears to the left of the rest of the letter. The letters of Units 12 and 13 belong in this group.
  3. Those that tend to be symmetrical and, by elimination, did not meet the criteria for inclusion into the above two groups. These are the letters in Units 10 and 11.

To place students in a level, the teacher administers the tests of Level B. Given evidence of mastery here, Level D tests are used; given the opposite–indications of non-mastery at Level B–Level A tests are used. Once a level of ability has been established, the teacher sets about implementing lessons to teach the higher order objectives–to teach the child to pass the tests.

Material and lessons designed to aid the teacher in teaching the objectives are also provided, although the teacher is encouraged to use, in addition, whatever appropriate materials she has available. Over one hundred supplemental instructional tapes and worksheets have been de­veloped along with twenty-six tapes and worksheets that can be used to test the child in the various objectives without requiring the constant presence of a tester.

Does the curriculum work? Inasmuch as teachers have success­fully taught these symbols for many generations, it is no great feat we seek to accomplish here. Use of the curriculum, however, alerts the teacher to each of her student’s competencies and needs; a child is less apt to be overlooked. To this extent, the curriculum is indeed useful. Table 15 shows, for both grades, the median number of objectives mas­tered at the beginning-of-year placement test, and the median number of additional objectives mastered during the four months that followed place­ment testing. (These data, too, are presented differently than the Vis­ual-motor and Auditory-motor because the structure is not organized as a linear sequence. It is probable that a child will be on one level in some units and on a different level in other units. To avoid confusion, the data merely represent total number of objectives; no attempt is made, in this presentation, to isolate individual units.) Again, the children’s progress is obvious.

Table 15

Median Total Number of Letters & Numerals Objectives Mastered

at September Placement Testing and the Median Number of

Additional Objectives Mastered in the Subsequent Four Months

The cumulative frequency ogives graphed from these data are shown in Figure 19. It is evident here that, although the kindergarten group has made progress, from September to January, it is still far be-low the level of competency shown by the first grade in September. This is a markedly different picture from that observed in the other three curricular components, where the kindergarten children, in January, were approximating, or surpassing, the first-grade children’s Septem­ber performance.

Figure 19

Figure19

Relative Distribution of Objectives Mastered in the Letters and Numerals Component

as Measured in September and January of the Same School Year.

The difference, in this component, is probably a function of the amount of time devoted to teaching the Letters & Numerals objectives. It is conceivable, however, that the curriculum activities do not teach the objectives as well as they should. End-of-year kindergarten data will provide more data and enable us to make a valid judgment.

SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS

The four goals that we set have, to at least some degree, been attained. Certain behavioral processes, that we called “perceptual skills,” have been identified as directly related to primary-grade achieve­ment. They have been described behaviorally, and it has been demon­strated that they can be taught to young children. Further it has been shown that when children are taught these basic information processing skills, transfer effects–in classroom behaviors–are evidenced. Finally, it has been shown that the teaching of perceptual skills can be managed in the classroom by using an organized testing and training program that recognizes individual differences among children.

Four behavioral structures have been described–components of the Perceptual Skills Curriculum. Our evidence indicates that mastery of the terminal objectives of these structures, by six- and seven-year-old children, predicts their competency in the basic information process­ing- skills that are presumed by the usual instructional programs of the first and second grades.

Competent visual-motor skills will indicate an ability to analyze and organize visual data according to spatial attributes- -to extract specific patterns of visual information from larger and more complex patterns. Evidence has been provided that relates this set of skills more closely to the tasks of beginning arithmetic than to reading.

Competent auditory-motor skills will indicate an ability to analyze and organize acoustical data- -to extract specific patterns of auditory information from greater and more complex patterns. Evidence has been offered that relates this set of skills more closely to the task of beginning reading than to arithmetic.

The direct value of competent general-motor skills is still de­batable. Clearly, they are useful. Facility in manipulating a pencil, and in controlling the speech production mechanism and the extraocular system, can hardly be considered as undesirable. Awkwardness is rarely a positive feature; social acceptance in a classroom often centers on such a factor. Thus, although there is still insufficient data that relates gen­eral motor skills with academic achievement, the component is concerned with desirable behaviors.

The fourth component- -Letters & Numerals–is more concerned with content than process. As such, it is not really devoted to percep­tual skills. The component is included, primarily, because it represents a very necessary set of primary-grade behaviors and, also, because the behaviors are such that they require the child to organize and relate vis­ual and acoustical information simultaneously. Visual-motor skills cen­ter on one’s ability to bring order to three-dimensional space. Auditory skills, on the other hand, are related to the dimension of time. Printing letters in an ordered manner–as, for example, in spelling–requires both types of organizational skills. Directionality, such as from left to right, insists on visual-motor directives. Sequencing letters so that they map accurately on the sounds requires auditory perceptual compe­tencies. Hence, learning to name and print the letters of the alphabet, and learning to arrange them in a left to right sequence, are representa­tive of basic integrative functions that are critical to satisfactory school performance. The child is learning to mediate visually the dimension of time (hear and print) and, in reverse, to mediate acoustically the dimen­sions of visual space (see and say). Thus, when he has acquired compe­tency in these behaviors, his chances of meeting the demands of the pri­mary academic programs should be enhanced.

Are there other critical variables–perceptual skills–that relate, in a different way, to classroom achievement? Probably. Those already identified and described in this paper do not seem to be sufficiently extensive nor adequately complex to account for as much of the variance in classroom achievement as we would like. In retrospect they may even seem trifling, compared to the time and effort devoted to the task by the staff of this Project and the many other LRDC Research Associates who have contributed valuable guidance. Thus far, however, no other impor­tant variables have been defined. Perhaps, as Simon (1969) states:

A man, viewed as a behaving system, is quite simple. The apparent complexity of his behavior over time is largely a reflection of the complexity of the environ­ment in which he finds himself (p. 25).

Is it possible that the child’s analysis and synthesis skills, initially chal­lenged by relatively uncomplicated visual and acoustical perceptual con­structs, become no more elaborate but, instead, become only more efficient- -that they apply the same behavioral processes to larger and mere abstract units of analysis? The question cannot now be answered; the prospect is an interesting one.


References

Gesell, A. Infant development. New York: Harper & Brothers, 1952.

Beck, I. L. , & Mitroff, D. D. Rationale and design of a primary grades reading system for an individualized classroom. Pittsburgh: Learning Research and Development Center, 1972. (LRDC Publi­cation 1972/4)

Bender, L. A visual motor Gestalt test and its clinical use. New York: American Orthopsychiatric Association, 1938. (Research Mono­graph No. 3)

Bereiter, C. , & Engelmann, S. Teaching disadvantaged children in the preschool. Englewood Cliffs, N. J. : Prentice-Hall, 1966.

Bruce, D. J. The analysis of word sounds by young children. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 1964, 34, 158-170.

Chalfont, J. C. , & Scheffelin, M. A. Central processing dysfunctions in children: A review of the research. Washington, D. C.: U. S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, 1969. (NINDS Monograph No. 9)

Clements, S. D. Minimal brain dysfunction, terminology and identifica­tion. Washington, D. C.: U. S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, 1966. (NINDB Monograph No. 3, Public Health Ser­vice Bulletin No. 1415)

Clements, S. D. Minimal brain dysfunction in children. Washington, D.C.: U. S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, 1969. (N&SDCP Monograph, Public Health Service Bulletin No. 2015)

Deutsch, M. The role of social class in language development and cogni­tion. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 1965, 25, 78-88.

Espenschade, A. S., & Eckert, H. M. Motor development. Columbus, Ohio: Charles E. Merrill, Inc., 1967.

Frierson, E. C. , & Barbe, B. B. Educating children with learning dis­abilities: Selected readings. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1967.

Frostig, M. Frostig Program for the Development of Visual Perception. Chicago: Follett Publishing Co. , 1964.

Gesell~ A. , Ilg, F. , & Bullis, G. Vision: Its development in infant and child. New York: Paul B. Hoeber, Inc. , 1949.

Gesell, A. , et al. The first five years of life: A guide to the study of the preschool child. New York: Harper & Row, 1940.

Claser, R. Adaptive education. Paper presented at the Conference on University Teaching and Learning, McGill University, Montreal, Canada, October 20-23, 1971.

Glaser, R. , & Nitko, A. J. Measurement in learning and instruction. In R.L. Thorndike (Ed.), Educational measurement. Washington, D. C.: American Council on Education, 1971. Pp. 601-622.

Held, R., & Hem, A. Movement-produced stimulation in the development of visually-guided behavior. Journal of Comparative and Psycho­logical Psychology, 1963, 56, 872-876.

Howard, I. P., & Templeton, W. B. Human spatial orientation. New York: Wiley, 1966.

llg, F., & Ames, L. School readiness. New York: Harper & Row, 1964.

Jastak, J. F., Bijou, S. W., & Jastak, S. R. Wide Range Achievement Test. Wilmington, Del.: Guidance Associates, 1965.

Kaluger, G. , & Kolson, C. Reading and learning disabilities. Columbus, Ohio: Charles E. Merrill, Inc. , 1969.

Kephart, N. C. The slow learner in the classroom. Columbus, Ohio: Charles E. Merrill, Inc., 1960.

Lenneberg, E. H. Biological foundations of language. New York: Wiley, 1967.

Luria, A. R. Higher cortical functions in man. New York: Basic Books, Inc., 1966.

McCarthy, J. J. , & McCarthy, J. F. Learning disabilities. Boston: Allyn and Bacon, 1969.

Pace Perceptual Program. McKeesport, Pa.: Pace School, 1968. (Mim­eographed report)

Piaget, J. Psychology of intelligence. Patterson, N. J. : Littlefield, Adams & Co., 1960.

Resnick, L. B. Design of an early learning curriculum. Pittsburgh: Learning Research and Development Center, 1967. (Working Paper 16)

Resnick, L. B. Open education: Some tasks for technology. Educational Technology, 1972, 12(1), 70-76.

Revusky, S. H. Some statistical treatments compatible with individual organism methodology. Journal of Experimental Analysis of Be­havior, 1967, 10, 319-330.

Richman, V., Rosner, J. , & Scott, R. Perceptual development and learning disabilities: Identification and remedial techniques. Pittsburgh: Pittsburgh Public Schools, Division of Mental Health Services, 1969.

Rosner, J. Visual Perceptual Survey. Oakmont, Pa.: Oakmont Public School District, 1966. (Mimeographed report)

Rosner, J. The design of an individualized Perceptual Skills Curriculum. Pittsburgh: Learning Research and Development Center, 1969. (Working Paper 53)

Rosner, J. The Design Board Program. Pittsburgh: Learning Research and Development Center, 1971. (a) (Publication 1971/7)

Rosner, J. Phonic analysis training and beginning reading skills. Paper presented at the Annual Meetings of the American Psychological Association, Washington, D. C. , September, 1971. (b) (Also LRDC Publication 1971/19)

Rosner, J. The Visual Analysis Test: An initial report. Pittsburgh: Learning Research and Development Center, 1971. (c) (Publica­tion 1971/22)

Rosner, J. , & Cooley, W. W. Changes in perceptual skills and early academic achievement. Unpublished paper, Learning Research and Development Center, 1971.

Rosner, J. , Levine, S., & Simon, D. Effects of Design Board training on the performance scale and subtests of the WPPSI. Paper pre­sented at the Annual Meetings of the American Educational Research Association, New York, February, 1971

Rosner, J. , Richman, V. , & Scott, R. H. The identification of~children with perceptual motor dysfunction. Pittsburgh: Learning Re search and Development Center, 1969. (Working Paper 47)

Rosner, J. , & Simon, D. The Auditory Analysis Test: An initial report. Pittsburgh: Learning Research and Development Center, 1971. (Also published in the Journal of Learning Disabilities, 1971, 4(7), 384-392)

Simon, H. The sciences of the artificial. Cambridge: The MIT Press, 1969.

Slingerland, B. Screening test for identifying children with specific lan­guage disabilities. Cambridge: Educators Publishing Service, 1962.

Starr, A. The Rutgers Drawing Test. New Brunswick, N. J.: Author, 1961.

Wechsler, D. The Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence. New York: The Psychological Corporation, 1967.

White, B. L. Experience and the development of motor mechanisms in infancy. In K. J. Connolly (Ed.), Mechanisms of motor skill development. New York: Academic Press, 1970. Pp. 95-136.

Witkin, H. A., Dyk, R. B., Faterson, H. F., Goodenough, D. R., & Karp, S. A. Psychological differentiation. New York: Wiley, 1962.

Zinchenko, V. P. Vicarious perceptual actions: A study of the motor com­ponents of recognition, immediate memory and thinking. In K. I. Connolly (Ed.), Mechanisms of motor skill development. New York: Academic Press, 1970. Pp. 225-242.

© 2011 Merrill D. Bowan, O.D. All rights reserved

Website Lillicotch.com

Powered by WordPress